Аннотация: Статья‚ опубликованная в журнале Journal of Sex Research, посвящена анализу супружеских измен в Америке. Ислледование проводилось в середине 90-х годов.
Database: Magazine &
Journal Articles
Personal Profile:
Off
List: List
One Saved: 0 items Saved in all lists:
0 items
Wiederman, Michael W. Extramarital
sex: prevalence and correlates in a national survey.
Journal of Sex Research v34, n2 (Spring, 1997):167 (8 pages). [Long
Display]
COPYRIGHT 1997 Society for the Scientific Study of Sex Inc.
In the current study, data from a nationally representative
sample of 884 men and 1,288 women (1994 General Social Survey, Davis
& Smith, 1994) who have ever been married were analyzed with
regard to incidence, prevalence, and correlates of extramarital sex
(EMS). Men were more likely than women to report ever engaging in
EMS (22.7% vs. 11.6%, p [is less than] .00001), yet, after
correcting the probability value for multiple tests, the apparent
gender difference regarding the proportion of respondents who had
EMS during the past year was not statistically significant (4.1% vs.
1.7%, p [is less than].008). Interestingly, there was no gender
difference in lifetime incidence among respondents younger than 40
years of age. Except for the oldest cohort, lifetime incidence of
EMS increased with age for men, whereas for women there was an
apparently curvilinear relationship such that lifetime incidence of
EMS was greatest among those 30-50 years of age. Those who have ever
been divorced, and those with greater attitudinal acceptance of EMS,
had higher incidence of EMS compared to those who have not been
divorced and those reporting greater disapproval of EMS. With regard
to possible gender differences, men and women who denied ever
engaging in EMS did not differ in their attitudes about EMS, just as
men and women who reported having experienced EMS did not differ in
their attitudes. The results are discussed in relation to previous
research and unanswered questions left for further investigation.
In their ground-breaking research, Kinsey and his colleagues
found that approximately one third of men and one fifth of women had
had extramarital sexual intercourse (see Kinsey, Pomeroy, &
Martin, 1948, p. 282; Kinsey, Pomeroy, Martin, & Gebhard, 1953,
p. 417). In the three decades following the Kinsey reports, numerous
sex surveys were conducted with convenience samples. In general, the
results of these subsequent surveys were either congruent with the
Kinsey group's findings or revealed a greater incidence of
extramarital sex (see Allgeier & Allgeier, 1995; Goettsch, 1994;
and Thompson, 1983, for reviews). In his review, Thompson (1983)
concluded, "Population parameters for extramarital coitus seem to be
at least 50% for married men, and the figure for married women is
rapidly approaching the same level" (p. 18).
The convenience samples upon which this conclusion was based were
surveyed during the 1970s and early 1980s. How do those earlier
rates of extramarital sex (EMS) compare to more recent findings?
Several large-scale sexual surveys recently have been conducted
using nationally representative samples. Although EMS was not the
focus of these surveys, they do contain relevant data. For example,
Smith (1991) reported results based on the 1989 General Social
Survey (GSS), a survey conducted annually with a representative
sample of American adults. Although the survey did not contain a
specific question about EMS, Smith ascertained the proportion of
married respondents who reported more than 1 sexual partner during
the previous 12 months. He found that only 1.5% of married Americans
(0.8% of women and 2.1% of men) apparently had had EMS in the past
year. These results prompted some to question whether having
multiple sex partners may be a "disappearing practice" among married
Americans (e.g., see Allgeier & Allgeier, 1995, p. 430).
Billy, Tanfer, Grady, and Klepinger (1993) reported results based
on a national sample of men aged 20-39 surveyed during 1991. They
found that 4% of currently married men reported more than 1 vaginal
intercourse partner during the past year. Forste and Tanfer (1996)
reported results based on a national sample of women aged 20-37
surveyed during 1991. They found that 4% of currently married women
reported having had EMS at some point.
Leigh, Temple, and Trocki (1993) reported results based on a
national sample of adults surveyed in 1990 with regard to number of
vaginal and/or anal intercourse partners during the past 30 days, 12
months, and 5 years. Among those respondents who were married during
each time period investigated, 1.2% reported more than 1 intercourse
partner during the past 30 days, 3.6% more than 1 partner during the
past 12 months, and 6.4% multiple partners during the past 5 years.
Regardless of the time span, about twice as many men as women
reported EMS. Similarly, Choi, Catania, and Dolcini (1994) reported
results based on a national sample of adults surveyed in 1990-91.
Among currently married respondents, 2.2% reported having more than
1 vaginal and/or anal intercourse partner during the preceding 12
months, with men about twice as likely as women to have had more
than 1 partner.
Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, and Michaels (1994) reported results of
their national survey and, although not presented by gender, 3.8% of
the currently married respondents reported more than 1 sex partner
during the past year (see p. 217). These authors also asked about
lifetime experience of EMS and found that 24.5% of ever-married men
and 15.0% of ever-married women reported having experienced EMS at
some point (see p. 216). A problematic issue in the Laumann et al.
study, however, was possible contamination of the EMS items, because
21% of respondents were interviewed with a child, spouse, or some
other person in the room (see p. 568). Years earlier, Johnson (1970)
found that men and women who completed questionnaires about sexual
experience in the same room as their spouse were less likely to
report EMS experience relative to married men and women who
completed the same questionnaire in separate rooms of the house. In
the Laumann et al. study, women were more likely than men to have
had another person in the room during participation in the
interview, and having a mate in the room during the interview may
have inhibited reports of multiple sex partners; 5% of respondents
interviewed with a spouse or sex partner in the room reported having
had more than 1 sex partner in the last year compared to 17% of
those interviewed without a partner present (see Laumann et al.,
1994, p. 568).
Most recently, in February 1994, Parade Magazine conducted a
telephone survey of 1,049 adults between the ages of 18 and 65
selected to represent the demographic characteristics of the United
States adult population (Clements, 1994). Of the married
respondents, 19% of the men and 15% of the women indicated that they
had had EMS. The prevalence of EMS was greatest within the oldest
cohort (23% among those ages 55-65).
In general, the results of these relatively recent surveys
suggest that between 15-25% of ever-married Americans report having
had EMS, but EMS occurs infrequently during any year (1.5-4.0%
incidence during the preceding 12 months). Also, it appears that men
are at least somewhat more likely than women to report EMS. Despite
these general findings, we currently have few answers to some rather
rudimentary questions about EMS. For example, how are basic
demographic factors such as age, ethnicity, size of the community in
which one resides, and history of divorce related, if at all, to
EMS? Within their sample of White males, Kinsey et al. (1948) found
that lifetime prevalence of EMS remained remarkably stable across
age groups but may have declined slightly with age (see p. 282),
whereas among their female sample, Kinsey et al. (1953) found that
the incidence of EMS rose with age, peaking by about age 40 (see p.
417). Laumann et al. (1994) found that lifetime prevalence of EMS
increased steadily with age for men but showed a curvilinear
relationship for women, such that likelihood of EMS was highest
among those ages 40-50 (see p. 216). In the Parade Magazine survey,
lifetime prevalence of EMS was highest among the older cohort (ages
55-65), but no distinction was made based on gender (Clements,
1994).
Even less is known about EMS as related to ethnicity, size of
community of residence, history of divorce, and EMS attitudes. Choi
et al. (1994) found that Black men and women had higher rates of EMS
during the preceding year relative to White men and women.
Similarly, in their sample of young adult women, Forste and Tanfer
(1996) found that Black women and Hispanic women had higher rates of
EMS relative to White women. With regard to size of city, Kinsey et
al. (1948) found that lifetime prevalence of EMS was higher among
men in urban environments compared to rural areas (see p. 456).
Also, in a national sample, Weis and Jurich (1985) found that
attitudes about EMS were uniquely related to size of community of
residence, even after controlling for ethnicity and premarital
sexual permissiveness; greater acceptance of EMS was demonstrated
among those residing in urban areas.
With regard to potential links between EMS attitudes and
experience, Thompson (1983) concluded, "There is little evidence to
suggest that [attitude about EMS] predicts behavior" (p. 17). Yet
Kinsey and his colleagues (1953) noted an apparent relationship
between EMS experience and attitudinal acceptance of the behavior
(see p. 416). The relationships between likelihood of EMS and age,
ethnicity, size of city of residence, divorce history, and EMS
attitudes are not clear. Also, it is important to consider patterns
of nonresponse to EMS survey items in that notably higher rates of
nonresponse by particular subsamples, may bias conclusions (see
Biggar & Melbye, 1992; Laumann et al., 1994, pp. 556-560;
Wiederman, 1993a).
The focus of the current study was investigation of potential
relationships between EMS and the several factors discussed
previously. Specifically, I sought to investigate: (a) the lifetime
prevalence of EMS among men and women in the United States as well
as the incidence of EMS during the past year; (b) potential
relationships between EMS and age, ethnicity, size of the community
in which one resides, history of divorce, and EMS attitudes; (c) the
degree of overlap between having ever engaged in EMS and having done
so during the past year; and (d) the extent to which nonresponse to
EMS questions might threaten the accuracy of EMS-prevalence
estimates. In the current study, aforementioned questions and issues
were investigated using data from a national sample--the 1994 GSS.
Method
Participants
The GSS is conducted annually by the National Opinion Research
Center (NORC) and consists of face-to-face interviews with adults
selected to represent the civilian household population of the
continental United States (see Davis & Smith, 1994). The sample
is limited to those who speak English and are at least 18 years of
age. Respondents in the current study were 884 men and 1,288 women
who participated in the 1994 GSS and indicated that they had ever
been married. Of these, 663 (75%) of the men were currently married,
whereas 760 (59%) of the women were currently married. The large
majority of men (85.7%) and women (85.8%) were White; 10.3% of men
and 10.7% of women were Black, and the remaining 4.0% of men and
3.5% of women were self-identified as belonging to another ethnic
group.
Measures
Demographics. In recruiting the sample for the GSS, ethnic
minorities are not oversampled, leaving relatively small groups of
non-White respondents. As such, ethnicity was coded as either White,
Black, or Other in the 1994 GSS data set. Survey research center
belt code referred to the size and type of community in which the
respondent resided and was coded from 1 (Central city of 1 of the 12
largest standard metropolitan areas) to 6 (Rural--counties having no
towns with 10, 000 or more people). Current marital status was coded
according to Never Married, Married, Separated, Divorced, or
Widowed. Additionally, ever-married respondents were asked, "Have
you ever been divorced or legally separated?" For more information
on coding of GSS variables, see Davis and Smith (1994).
Sexual experience. Immediately after the face-to-face interview,
participants were given a brief, self-administered questionnaire
regarding their sexual experience, which they sealed in an envelope
before returning it to the interviewer. The questionnaire items used
in the current study included the open-ended question "How many sex
partners have you had in the last twelve months?" as well as "Was
one of the partners your husband or wife or regular sex partner?"
and "Have you ever had sex with someone other than your husband or
wife while you were married?"
Among the currently married respondents, I determined the
occurrence of EMS during the past year using the following schema. I
categorized those respondents who either reported no sexual partners
during the past year (i.e., responded with a 0 to the question "How
many sex partners have you had in the last twelve months?") or
reported having had one partner, including their mate (i.e.,
responded with a 1 to the question "How many sex partners have you
had in the last twelve months?" and responded "yes" to the question
"Was one of the partners your husband or wife or regular sex
partner?") as having been maritally monogamous. I categorized those
respondents who reported having more than one sexual partner (i.e.,
responded with [is greater than] 1 to the question "How many sex
partners have you had in the last twelve months?") or having had one
partner who was not their mate (i.e., responded with a 1 to the
question "How many sex partners have you had in the last twelve
months?" and responded "no" to the question "Was one of the partners
your husband or wife or regular sex partner?") as having experienced
EMS during the past year.
This coding process is less than perfect, as it is possible that
a respondent may have had only one sex partner, but not his or her
spouse, and considered that EMS partner as a "regular sex partner"
in the response to the question, thereby appearing maritally
monogamous according to the answers. Still, the items used here to
determine rates of EMS during the preceding year represent an
improvement over past studies (e.g., Billy et al., 1993; Choi et
al., 1994; Leigh et al., 1993) that determined the incidence of EMS
through the total number of sex partners without discerning whether
the respondent's spouse was included among them.
Another potential problem with the sexual experience items used
in the GSS has to do with interpretation of the terms sex partner
and had sex. It is possible for some respondents to interpret these
terms in idiosyncratic ways (e.g., see Wellings, Field, Johnson,
& Wadsworth, 1994, pp. 18-19). Also, self-reports of sexual
activity are prone to distortion because of faulty recall (Catania,
Gibson, Chitwood, & Coates, 1990; Jaccard & Wan, 1995). This
was considered less of a problem in the current study, as the exact
number of sex partners was not a variable of interest but rather
whether married respondents had more than one partner during the
past year or had ever had EMS. Given the cultural disapproval
regarding EMS (see Table 4 as well as Laumann et al., 1994, p. 599),
EMS experience is probably a very salient event for. most Americans
and hence a variable less susceptible to recall bias.
Table 4 Attitudes About the Acceptability of Extramarital Sex
(EMS) as a Function of Gender and EMS Experience
Men
Ever Had EMS? Yes No
n % n %
Acceptability of EMS
Always wrong 76 57.1(A,C) 373 83.8(A,D)
Almost always wrong 29 21.8 49 11.0
Wrong only sometimes 19 14.3 19 4.3
Not wrong at all 9 6.8 4 0.9
100 445 100
Women
Yes No
n % n %
Acceptability of EMS
Always wrong 58 56.3(B,C) 625 86.9(B,D)
Almost always wrong 24 23.3 55 7.6
Wrong only sometimes 16 15.5 33 4.6
Not wrong at all 5 4.9 6 0.8
Totals 103 100 719 100
(A) Men with EMS experience vs. men without EMS experience,
[X.sup.2] (3, N = 578) = 49.52, p [is less than].00001
(B) Women with EMS experience vs. women without EMS experience,
[X.sup.2] (3, N = 822) = 62. 11, p [is less than] .00001
(C) Men with EMS experience vs. women with EMS experience,
[X.sup.2] (3, N = 236) = .48, p [is less than] .93
(D) Men without EMS experience vs. women without EMS experience,
[X.sup.2] (3, N = 1, 164) = 3.86, p [is less than] .28
EMS attitudes. Attitude toward acceptability of EMS was measured
with the item, "What is your opinion about a married person having
sexual relations with someone other than the marriage partner--is it
always wrong, almost always wrong, wrong only sometimes, or not
wrong at all?" Accordingly, scores ranged from 1 to 4, with higher
scores indicating more permissive attitudes.
Results
I performed 19 statistical tests. To maintain an experimentwise
error rate of .05, the individual probability value used to
determine statistical significance was corrected using the standard
Bonferroni procedure (Rosenthal & Rubin, 1984). Accordingly, the
results of each test were deemed statistically significant if p [is
less than] .003 (i.e., .05 / 19 = .003).
The lifetime prevalence of EMS, as well as the incidence of EMS
during the past year, is presented in Table 1 by gender, ethnicity,
and history of divorce. Overall, men were more likely than women to
report ever having had EMS. However, with regard to EMS during the
preceding 12 months, the apparent gender difference was not
statistically significant after correcting the probability value, as
noted previously. Because of the small number of respondents who
were not White or Black, only these two groups were compared. After
I corrected the probability value, there were no differences between
White and Black men, or White and Black women, with regard to
lifetime incidence of EMS. However, Black men were more likely than
White men to report having engaged in EMS during the past year, and
the same ethnic difference was found for women.
Table 1 Incidence of Extramarital Sex (EMS) By Gender, Ethnicity,
and Divorce Experience
Men
n % Yes Nonresponse
Ever Have EMS?
Total sample 884 22.7(A) 2.9
Whites 758 21.4(B) 2.6
Blacks 91 33.0(B) 5.5
Other ethnicity 35 25.7 2.9
Ever divorced 304 37.5(D) 2.6
Never divorced 579 15.0(D) 3.1
EMS During the
Past Year?
Total sample 663 4.1(F) 1.4
Whites 571 2.9(G) 1.0
Blacks 55 12.1(G) 5.2
Other ethnicity 28 10.7 0.0
Ever divorced 167 6.6(I) 1.8
Never divorced 495 3.2(I) 1.2
Women
n % Yes Nonresponse
Ever Have EMS?
Total sample 1,288 11.6(A) 2.5
Whites 1,105 11.1(C) 2.0
Blacks 138 15.9(C) 6.5
Other ethnicity 45 11.1 2.2
Ever divorced 411 20.2(E) 2.4
Never divorced 876 7.6(E) 2.5
EMS During the
Past Year?
Total sample 760 1.7(F) 1.3
Whites 675 1.2(H) 1.2
Blacks 58 6.9(H) 3.4
Other ethnicity 27 3.7 0.0
Ever divorced 166 3.0(J) 1.8
Never divorced 593 1.3(J) 1.2
(A) Men vs. women, [X.sup.2] (1, N = 2,114) = 48.55, p < .0000
1
(B) White men vs. Black men, [X.sup.2] (1, N = 829) = 7.2 1, p
< .008
(C) White women vs. Black women, [X.sup.2] (1, N = 1,212) = 3.55,
p & .06
(D) Ever-divorced men vs. never-divorced men, [X.sup.2] (1, N =
857) = 57.12, p < .00001
(E) Ever-divorced women vs. never-divorced women, [X.sup.2] (1, N
= 1,255) = 42.83, p < .00001
(F) Men vs. women, [X.sup.2] (1, N = 1,404) = 7.24, p < .008
(G) White men vs. Black men, [X.sup.2] (1, N = 626) = 12.94, p
< .4
(H) White women vs. Black women, [X.sup.2] (1, N = 723) = 11. 18,
p & .0009
(I) Ever-divorced men vs. never-divorced men, [X.sup.2] (1, N =
653) = 3.66, p < .06
J) Ever-divorced women vs. never-divorced women, [X.sup.2] (1,
N749) = 2.17, p < .15
Lifetime rates of EMS were twice as high among those who had been
divorced or legally separated, compared to respondents who had never
divorced or separated, and this difference was statistically
significant for both men and women. Similarly, rates of EMS during
the past year were about twice as high among those who had been
divorced or legally separated, compared to respondents who had never
divorced/separated, but this difference was not statistically
significant for men or women. Additionally, I investigated potential
relationships between EMS experience and size of community in which
the respondent resided. For both men and women, size of community
was unrelated to having experienced EMS during the past year (r =
.03, p [is greater than] .05, and r = .04, p [is greater than] .05,
respectively) or during one's lifetime (r = -.0 1, p [is greater
than] .05, and r = .03, p [is greater than] .05, respectively).
The incidence of EMS is presented in Table 2 by gender and age.
With the exception of men aged 70 and older, lifetime prevalence of
EMS appeared to increase with age among men. The point-biserial
correlation between age and ever having had EMS was statistically
significant (r = .17, p [is less than] .00 1). However, after
partialing out the linear component of the relationship between age
and lifetime EMS experience, the curvilinear component was also
statistically significant (partial r = -.12, p [is less than] .00
1). So, for men, the lifetime incidence of EMS increased with age up
to the oldest age group at which point the incidence decreased.
There was no relationship between age and incidence of EMS during
the past year.
Table 2
Incidence of Extramarital Sex (EMS) By Gender and Age
Men
%
n % Yes Nonresponse
Ever Have EMS?
20-29 67 14.9 0.0
30-39 224 14.3 2.7
40-49 232 29.3 2.6
50-59 150 28.7 2.7
60-69 97 34.0 4.1
70 and older 114 13.2 5.3
Had EMS in the
Past Year?
20-29 61 3.3 0.0
30-39 181 7.2 2.8
40-49 176 3.4 0.6
50-59 98 3.1 0.0
60-69 70 1.4 1.4
70 and older 77 2.6 2.6
Women
%
n % Yes Nonresponse
Ever Have EMS?
20-29 129 10.9 2.3
30-39 323 14.2 0.9
40-49 286 19.3 0.3
50-59 202 10.9 4.0
60-69 158 7.6 3.8
70 and older 188 0.5 5.3
Had EMS in the
Past Year?
20-29 97 3.1 2.1
30-39 226 2.7 0.4
40-49 173 1.7 1.2
50-59 116 0.9 1.7
60-69 88 0.0 1.1
70 and older 59 0.0 3.4
For women, there was an apparently curvilinear relationship
between age and lifetime incidence of EMS. The point-biserial
correlation between age and ever having had EMS was statistically
significant (r = -.13, p [is less than] .001). However, after
partialing out the linear component of the relationship between age
and lifetime EMS experience, the curvilinear component was also
statistically significant (partial r = -.10, p [is less than] .005).
The apparent relationship between age and EMS for women was as
follows: Women in their twenties demonstrated the same likelihood of
lifetime EMS as did women in their fifties, and the highest
incidence occurred among women in their forties. Women aged 60 and
older were least likely to report ever having engaged in EMS, and
this was particularly true for women aged 70 and older (only 1 of
these 188 respondents reported lifetime EMS experience). Considering
men and women under the age of 40, there was no gender difference in
likelihood of reporting lifetime experience with EMS (14.4% vs.
13.3%, respectively), [X.sup.2](1, N = 743) = .24, p [is less than]
.63. With regard to incidence of EMS during the past 12 months, the
incidence steadily decreased with age for women (r = -.08, p [is
less than].05).
How do reports of ever having engaged in EMS compare to reports
of having experienced EMS during the preceding 12 months? The
results of cross-tabulating responses to these two items are
presented in Table 3 for men and women who were currently married.
The large majority of currently married men (78%) and women (88%)
consistently denied EMS both during the past year and during their
lifetime. Of those who reported ever engaging in EMS, only 16.2% of
the men and 12.3% of the women had apparently done so during the
past 12 months. For the most part, those who failed to respond to
one of the indices of EMS denied EMS involvement on the other index.
Curiously, nine men and five women denied ever engaging in EMS yet
appeared not to be maritally monogamous during the past year.
Unfortunately, the limited nature of the GSS data did not allow for
explanation of the inconsistent responses. Still, the relatively
small number of men and women who were inconsistent in their
responses do not appear to pose a threat to the overall prevalence
estimates.
Table 3 Number of Currently Married Men (n = 663) and Women (n =
760) Who Reported Ever Having Engaged in Extramarital Sex (EMS) and
Having Engaged in EMS During the Past Year
Had EMS During the Past Year?
Men
No
Yes No Response
n % n % n %
Ever had EMS?
Yes 18 2.7 93 14.0 0 0
No 9 1.4 517 78.0 8 1.2
No response 0 0 17 2.6 1 0.2
Women
No
Yes No Response
n % n % n %
Ever had EMS?
Yes 8 1.1 57 7.5 0 0
No 5 0.7 669 88.0 10 1.3
No response 0 0 11 1.4 0 0
How did attitudinal acceptance of EMS compare with actual
experience? The attitudinal item regarding EMS was administered to a
randomly selected majority subsample of the larger GSS sample (see
Davis & Smith, 1994), resulting in responses on both the
attitudinal item and lifetime experience item by 578 men and 822
women. The distribution of responses to the attitudinal item is
presented in Table 4 by gender and lifetime EMS experience. Men and
women who had engaged in EMS were less likely to disapprove of EMS
than were their peers without EMS experience. With regard to
possible gender differences, men and women who denied ever engaging
in EMS did not differ in their attitudes about EMS, just as men and
women who reported having experienced EMS did not differ in their
attitudes.
Discussion
Laumann et al. (1994) found that 24.5% of men and 15.0% of women
in their sample reported ever having experienced EMS. In the current
study the respective percentages are consistent, although slightly
lower, at 22.7% and 11.6%. Similarly, in the current study the
incidence of EMS during the preceding year was 4.1% of currently
married men and 1.7% of the currently married women. These rates are
twice as high as those reported by Smith (1991) but are remarkably
similar to the results found in other recent samples (Billy et al.,
1993; Choi et al., 1994; Laumann et al., 1994; Leigh et al., 1993).
Interestingly, when we consider the entire sample, the results of
the current study are congruent with past research in demonstrating
a substantial gender difference in lifetime incidence of EMS.
However, when we consider respondents less than 40 years of age, the
gender difference disappears. Either the previous "double standard"
with regard to involvement in EMS does not exist among those from
the younger generations, or there is a response bias such that older
women are least likely to admit EMS experience they have had.
Indeed, the greatest rates of nonresponse to the EMS items occurred
among the oldest respondents (male and female), which is consistent
with rates of nonresponse to sexual experience items in general
(Wiederman, 1993a). It is difficult to ascertain, however, whether
these nonresponders were motivated by a desire to "hide something"
through omission or simply left the items blank because they deemed
them not applicable.
Further research on the meaning of nonresponse to sexual
experience items is warranted (Wiederman, 1993a), yet in the current
study, rates of nonresponse to the EMS were relatively low. The
potential effects of distortion and deceit on reported EMS
experience are more difficult to ascertain. Because a large majority
of respondents strongly disapproved of EMS (see Table 4), the
incidence figures reported here should be considered lower bound
estimates for American adults.
For men, lifetime rates of EMS appeared to increase with age
(with the exception of the oldest cohort), whereas for women, there
appeared to be a curvilinear relationship such that the greatest
lifetime incidence of EMS occurred among women between the ages of
30 and 50. Yet incidence of EMS during the past year declined
steadily with age for women and was rather consistent across age
groups for men. Based on these data alone, any explanation for these
gender differences is merely speculation. However, response bias
notwithstanding, it may be that there is a cohort effect for women,
such that those respondents born in the latter half of the 20th
century were more likely than women born before that time to find
EMS possible and/or acceptable. During the last several decades,
increasing numbers of women worked outside of the home, and many
sexual attitudes underwent significant change (Smith, 1990). Also,
the finding that younger married women were less likely to have had
EMS relative to middle aged married women is consistent with earlier
observations that women seek extra marital involvement later in
their marriages than do men (e.g., see Kinsey et al., 1953).
In contrast to the finding on the lifetime incidence of EMS,
recent involvement in EMS by women was generally limited to the
youngest women in the sample. It may be that older women, relative
to older men, have a greater commitment to the marriage with the
greater number of years invested. This trend may also be related to
a double standard of aging, in that women's attractiveness to men is
heavily dependent on physical features related to relative youth,
whereas this is not the case regarding men's attractiveness to women
(Buss, 1994). Hence older women are commonly judged less attractive
compared to younger women (Deutsch, Zalenski, & Clark, 1986;
Henss, 1991; Mathes, Brennan, Haugen, & Rice, 1985). This
perception results in men desiring female sexual partners who are
progressively younger than themselves as the men age (Clements,
1996; Wiederman, 1993b). Also, there are many more unattached older
women than there are unattached older men (U.S. Bureau of the
Census, 1993). For these reasons, older married women probably have
fewer opportunities for EMS than do their male peers or relatively
younger women (and may be more committed to the marriage as a
result). This double standard of aging would also explain why, for
men in the current sample, lifetime rates of EMS increased with age
(a cumulative effect) and rates of EMS during the past year remained
relatively stable across age groups (opportunity for men remains the
same, or actually increases because of imbalanced gender ratios,
with advancing age).
In contrast to attitudes about EMS (Weis & Jurich, 1985) as
well as Kinsey et al.'s (1948) data, EMS experience was unrelated to
size of community in which the respondent resided. Likelihood of EMS
was related, however, to ethnicity, with Black men and women being
somewhat more likely than White men and women to have engaged in EMS
during the past year This finding is consistent with research on
ethnic differences in general sexual experience (e.g., Rushton &
Bogaert, 1987, 1988; Seidman & Rieder, 1994), as well as EMS
(Choi et al., 1994; Forste & Tanfer, 1996), and may be due at
least partly to ethnic differences in the gender ratio. That is,
there is a relative shortage of available unmarried men in the Black
community (Tucker & Taylor, 1989); thus, married Black men may
have greater opportunities for sexual relations with single Black
women.
In contrast to the conclusion by Thompson (1983), attitudinal
acceptance of EMS was related to EMS experience in the current
national sample. A slight majority of men and women who had had EMS
still indicated that it was "always wrong," yet about 20% of those
with EMS experience compared to about 5% of those without EMS
experience indicated that EMS was wrong "only some times" or "not at
all." The direction of the relationship between EMS attitudes and
experiences is unknown based on these cross-sectional data. That is,
we do not know if more accepting attitudes are responsible for an
increased likelihood of engaging in EMS, or if EMS experience
results in greater attitudinal acceptance for some participants.
Also, we do not know what to make of those respondents who evidenced
a distinct attitude-behavior discrepancy. Because EMS attitudes were
measured with a single item, appropriate caution should be exercised
in interpreting the results for EMS attitudes, as the measure might
have been relatively unreliable.
To speculate, it could be that those who had EMS experience and
strongly disapproved of EMS are participants for whom EMS
involvement "went bad" and exacted some toll on their marriage or
social standing. Although this is a plausible sequence of events, in
one study of divorced adults, EMS was generally seen by the
respondents to be a result, rather than a cause, of marital discord
(Spanier & Margolis, 1983). In the current study, likelihood of
EMS was greater among respondents who had been divorced or legally
separated, compared to respondents who had never experienced
separation or divorce. Unfortunately, given the limited information
in the GSS, it is impossible to ascertain the role of EMS in divorce
for these respondents. It may be that EMS involvement increased the
probability of divorce, EMS was a result of a disintegrating
marriage (and may have occurred after separation from the spouse),
or a third factor, such as decreased commitment to the institution
of marriage, resulted in an apparent relationship ("spurious
correlation") between the experiences of EMS and divorce. Support
for a direct relationship between having EMS and being divorced by
one's spouse includes strong disapproval of EMS in this culture (see
Table 4) and the previous finding that, across cultures, sexual
infidelity is the most commonly stated reason for divorce (Betzig,
1989).
Across studies, many of which employed college students, men
generally exhibit less disapproval of EMS than do women (Oliver
& Hyde, 1993). In the current study, when considered separately
according to whether the respondent had experienced EMS, men and
women did not differ in their EMS attitudes. It may be that when one
considers married adults apart from single adults, there is simply
less discrepancy between men and women in their views on EMS. Also,
perhaps past studies on gender differences in EMS attitudes were
confounded by gender differences in rates of EMS experience. As EMS
experience is related to greater acceptance of EMS (see Table 4),
samples in which men are significantly more likely than women to
have such experience may be likely to exhibit corresponding gender
differences in attitudes.
Although the rates of EMS in the current study are lower than in
past studies using convenience samples, a significant minority of
Americans have experienced EMS and continue to do so. In many ways,
the results of the current study raise at least as many questions as
provide answers. As suggested in earlier writings (e.g.' Bell,
Turner, & Rosen, 1975; Thompson, 1983, 1984), further research
is needed to understand individual differences in the experience of
EMS, the conditions under which EMS occurs, and the roles EMS plays
in people's lives.
References
Allgeier, A. R., & Allgeier, E. R. (1995). Sexual
interactions (4th ed.). Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath.
Bell, R. R., Turner, S., & Rosen, L. (1975). A multivariate
analysis of female extramarital coitus. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 37, 375-384.
Betzig, L. (1989). Causes of conjugal dissolution: A
cross-cultural study. Current Anthropology, 30, 654-676.
Biggar, R. J., & Melbye, M. (1992). Responses to anonymous
questionnaires concerning sexual behavior: A method to examine
potential bias. American Journal of Public Health, 82, 1506-1512.
Billy, J. O. G., Tanfer, K., Grady, W. R., & Mepinger, D. H.
(1993). The sexual behavior of men in the United States. Family
Planning Perspectives, 25, 52-60.
Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human
mating. New York: Basic Books.
Catania, J. A., Gibson, D. R., Chitwood, D. D., & Coates, T.
J. (1990). Methodological problems in AIDS behavioral research:
Influences on measurement error and participation bias in studies of
sexual behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 339-362.
Choi, K. H., Catania, J. A., & Dolcini, M. M. (1994).
Extramarital sex and HIV risk behavior among U.S. adults: Results
from the national AIDS behavioral survey. American Journal of Public
Health, 84, 2003-2007.
Clements, M. (1994, August 7). Sex in America today: A new
national survey reveals how our attitudes are changing. Parade
Magazine, 4-6.
Clements, M. (1996, March 17). Sex after 65. Parade Magazine,
4-7.
Davis, J. A., & Smith, T. W. (1994). General social surveys,
1972-1994: Cumulative codebook. Chicago: National Opinion Research
Center.
Deutsch, F. M., Zalenski, C. M., & Clark, M. E. (1986). Is
there a double standard of aging? Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 16, 771-785.
Forste, R., & Tanfer, K. (1996). Sexual exclusivity among
dating, cohabiting, and married women. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 58, 33-47.
Goettsch, S. L. (1994). Extramarital sex. In V. L. Bullough &
B. Bullough (Eds.), Human sexuality: An encyclopedia (pp. 194-199).
New York: Garland.
Henss, R. (1991). Perceiving age and attractiveness in facial
photographs. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 933-946.
Jaccard, J., & Wan, C. K. (1995). A paradigm for studying the
accuracy of self-reports of risk behavior relevant to AIDS:
Empirical perspectives on stability, recall bias, and transitory
influences. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1831-1858.
Johnson, R. (1970). Extramarital sexual intercourse: A
methodological note. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 32,
279-282.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual
behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: W B. Saunders.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, R H.
(1953). Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadephia: W. B.
Saunders.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S.
(1994). The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in
the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Leigh, B. C., Temple, M. T., & Trocki, K. F. (1993). The
sexual behavior of U.S. adults: Results from a national survey.
American Journal of Public Health, 83, 1400-1408.
Mathes, E. W., Brennan, S. M., Haugen, P, M., & Rice, H. B.
(1985). Ratings of physical attractiveness as a function of age.
Journal of Psychology, 125, 157-168.
Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in
sexuality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29-51.
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1984). Multiple contrasts and
ordered Bonferroni procedures. Journal of Educational Psychology,
76, 1028-1034.
Rushton, J. R, & Bogaert, A. F. (1987). Race differences in
sexual behavior: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Journal of
Research in Personality, 21, 529-551.
Rushton, J. P, & Bogaert, A. F. (1988). Race versus social
class differences in sexual behavior: A follow-up test of the r1K
dimension. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 259-272.
Seidman, S. N., & Rieder, R. 0. (1994). A review of sexual
behavior in the United States. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151,
330-341.
Smith, T. W. (1990). The sexual revolution? Public Opinion
Quarterly, 54, 415-435.
Smith, T. W (1991). Adult sexual behavior in 1989: Number of
partners, frequency of intercourse and risk of AIDS. Family Planning
Perspectives, 23, 102-107.
Spanier, G. B., & Margolis, R. L. (1983). Marital separation
and extramarital sexual behavior. The Journal of Sex Research, 19,
23-48.
Thompson, A. R (1983). Extramarital sex: A review of the research
literature. The Journal of Sex Research, 19, 1-22.
Thompson, A. P. (1984). Emotional and sexual components of
extramarital relations. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46,
35-42.
Tucker, M. B., & Taylor, R. J. (1989). Demographic correlates
of relationship status among Black Americans. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 51, 655-665.
U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1993). Statistical abstract of the
United States, 1993 (113th ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Weis, D. L., & Juirich, J. (1985). Size of community of
residence as a predictor of attitudes toward extramarital sex.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 47, 173-178.
Wellings, K, Field, J., Johnson, A. M., & Wadsworth, J.
(1994). Sexual behavior in Britain: The National Survey of Sexual
Attitudes and Lifestyles. New York: Penguin Books.
Wiederman, M. W. (1993a). Demographic and sexual characteristics
of nonresponders to sexual experience items in a national survey.
The Journal of Sex Research, 30, 2735.
Wiederman, M. W. (1993b). Evolved gender differences in mate
preferences: Evidence from personal advertisements. Ethology and
Sociobiology, 14, 331-352.
Manuscript accepted August 5, 1996
I express appreciation to Stephen Johnson Ph.D., for assistance
in accessing the 1994 GSS data set and to Carri Maynard and three
anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and suggestions regarding
earlier versions of this article. Address correspondence to Michael
Wiederman, Ph.D., Department of Psychological Science, Ball State
University, Muncie, IN 47306-0520. Telephone: 317-2851690. Fax:
317-285-8980. E-mail: 00MWWIEDERMA@BSU.EDU.
Michael W. Wiederman, Ph.D. Department of Psychological Science,
Ball State University